Reading Efficiency in Native English-Speaking and English-as-a-Second-Language Children: The Role of Oral
Proficiency and UnderlyingCognitive-Linguistic Processes
Esther Geva
University of Toronto
Zohreh Yaghoub Zadeh
Canadian Council on Learning
Theresearchexaminedtheextenttowhich(a)Grade2English-as-a-second-lan-guage(ESL)andEnglish-as-a-first-language(EL1)childrenresembleeachotheronwordandtextreadingefficiencyand(b)whetherindividualdifferencesinwordandtextreadingefficiencyinthetwolanguagegroupscanbeunderstoodintermsofsim-ilarunderlyingcomponentprocesses.Despiteanorallanguageproficiencyadvan-tageintheEL1group,noEL1advantageexistedonanyofthecognitiveandreadingmeasures.Orallanguageproficiency,phonologicalawareness,rapidautomatizednaming,andaccuratewordrecognitionweresignificantpredictorsofwordandtextefficiencyintheESLgroup.OnlyrapidautomatizednamingandwordrecognitionweresignificantintheEL1group.Overall,withtheexceptionofEnglish-languageoralproficiencyskills,EL1andESLprofilesofthreeefficiencysubgroups(poorde-coders, low efficiency, and high efficiency) were highly similar.
Thegoalofthisarticleistoinvestigatethedevelopmentofwordandtextreadingeffi-ciencyinyoungchildrenlearningtoreadintheirsecondlanguage(L2).BecausealmostnoresearchhasbeenconductedonthedevelopmentoffluencyandefficiencyinL2read-ing,webeginbyprovidingabriefoverviewofthefirstlanguage(L1)-basedliterature.
CorrespondenceshouldbesenttoEstherGeva,TheDepartmentofHumanDevelopmentandAp-pliedPsychology,OntarioInstituteforStudiesinEducationoftheUniversityofToronto,Toronto,On-tario, Canada M5S 1V6. E-mail: egeva@oise.utoronto.ca
32
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
Noconsensusexistsinthereadingliteraturewithregardtotheconceptofread-ingfluency(Wolf&Katzir-Cohen,2001).Termssuchasfluency,efficiency,andautomaticityoverlaptosomedegreeandareoftenusedassynonyms(NationalReadingPanel,2000).Someusethetermautomaticitytorefertovariousaspectsofwordidentificationthatcanbeprocessedwithlittleeffortandattention.Theoriesofautomaticity(LaBerge&Samuels,1974)assumethataccuracydevelopsbeforespeedandthatanefficientreaderisonewhocanreadwordsbothaccuratelyandfast.Theoriesofverbalefficiency(Perfetti,1985)emphasizetheimportanceofef-fectivelexicalretrievalprocessesandtheirimpactonindividualdifferencesinreadingcomprehension.Researchershavesuggestedthatcomprehensioncanbeenhancedwhenlexicalaccessprocessesareexecutedefficientlyandautomati-cally,sothatcognitive-processingresourcesarenotovertaxed(Bowers,Golden,Kennedy,&Young,1994;Bowers&Wolf,1993;Carver,1997;LaBerge&Samuels,1974;Perfetti,1985;Shany&Biemiller,1995;Stanovich,1992).TheNationalReadingPanel(2000)expandedtheconceptofreadingfluencytoinclude“theabilitytogroupwordsappropriatelyintomeaningfulgrammaticalunitsforin-terpretation”(pp.3–6).Inthisarticleweusethetermwordreadingefficiencytore-fertoaccurateandfastreadingofisolatedwords,andtextreadingefficiencytore-fer to accurate and fast reading of text.
Recentmodelsespouseamoredynamic,developmental,andcomponentialap-proachtothestudyofreadingfluency(e.g.,Berninger,Abbot,Billingsly,&Nagy,2001;Kame’enui,Simmons,Good,&Harn,2001;Levy,2000;Wolf&Katzir-Co-hen,2001).KuhnandStahl(2000)discussedhowthedevelopmentofreadingflu-encyrelatestothestagesofdevelopmentdescribedbyChall(1996).WolfandKatzir-Cohensuggestedthatintheearlystagesofreadingdevelopmentfluencyentailsthegradualdevelopmentofaccurateandautomaticexecutionoflowerlevelcomponentsinvolvingorthographic,phonological,lexical,morphological,andsyntacticskills.Tobeefficientreaders,childrenneedtoincreasetheirrepertoireoflargeorthographicunitsthatareeasilyaccessiblefrommemoryandtocarryouttheselowerleveloperationswithspeed.Oncereadershavedevelopedefficiencywiththesebasicaspectsofreadingandworddecodingbecomeseffortlessandfast,textreadingefficiencyisreflectedinparalinguisticfeaturessuchasprosody.Effi-cienttextreadingallowsfortheallocationofattentionalresourcestohigherlevelreading skills involved in comprehension.
WolfandKatzir-Cohenmaintainedthatdysfluentreadingcanbetheresultofimpairmentinanycomponentprocess(Meyer&Felton,1999;Wolf,Bowers,&Biddle,2000).Overall,lessskilledL1readersaredescribedintheL1readingliter-atureasrecognizingprintedwordsmoreslowlythanskilledreaders(Biemiller,1977/78;Carver,1997;Denckla&Rudel,1976;Ehri,1998;Manis,Seidenberg,Doi,McBride-Chang,&Patterson,1996;Perfetti,1985;Torgesen,2001;Wagneretal.,1997).Twounderlyingprocessesthathavebeenimplicatedinefficientwordreadingarephonologicalawarenessandnamingspeed.Sometheoreticiansmain-
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
33
tainthatnamingspeedandphonologicalawarenesstapcommonunderlyingpho-nologicalprocesses(e.g.,Wagneretal.,1997).Othersmaintainthatnamingspeedmayinvolveproblemswithadistinctphonologicalandvisualtimingmechanismnecessaryforestablishingunitizedorthographicandphonologicalcodes(e.g.,Bowers&Wolf,1993;Breznitz,2001,2002;Wolf&Bowers,1999;seeSavage,2004,forareview).Notwithstandingthisunresolvedtheoreticaldebate,L1-basedstudiesonthespeedofnamingvariousstimuliindicatethatindividualdifferencesinspeedofletter,digit,orwordnamingpredictfluentreadinginL1(Kirbyetal.,2003;Stage,Sheppard,Davidson,&Browning,2001;Wimmer,Maryinger,&Landerl, 2000; Young & Bowers, 1995).
READING DEVELOPMENT IN L2
ResearchonL2readingdevelopmentinL2childrenisnotasextensiveastheliter-atureonL1readingdevelopment.However,inthelastdecadethedatabaseonL2readingdevelopmenthasexpanded.Ithasshownthat(a)accuracyindexesofwordrecognitionandspellingskillsinyoungL2childrenareoftenidenticaltothoseofL1children(e.g.,Geva,YaghoubZadeh,&Schuster,2000;Lesaux&Siegel,2004;Lipka,2003;Wade-Woolley&Siegel,1997;Wang&Geva,2003);(b)cog-nitive-linguisticcomponentssuchasphonologicalawarenessandrapidautoma-tizednaming(RAN)canbemeasuredreliablyandutilizedtopredictperformanceonwordreadingskillsinL1andL2childrenalike(e.g.,Chiappe&Siegel,1999;Comeau,Cormier,Grandmaison,&Lacroix,1999;Durgunoglu,Nagy,&Hancin-Bhatt,1993;Gevaetal.,2000;Gottardo,Yan,Siegel,&Wade-Woolley,2001;Lesaux&Siegel,2003;Lindsey,Manis,&Bailey,2003;Wade-Woolley&Siegel,1997);and(c)onceindividualdifferencesinphonemicawarenessandrapidnaminghavebeentakenintoaccount,orallanguageproficiencyskillsdonotaddsubstantiallytothevarianceinaccuratebasicreadingskillsinL2children(Arab-Moghaddam&Sénéchal,2001;Durgunogluetal.,1993;Geva&Siegel,2000; Geva et al., 2000; Gholamain & Geva, 1999; Lindsey et al., 2003).
AlthoughL2oralproficiencymaynotmakeauniquecontributionoverandaboveotherbasicreadingcomponentstoaccuracyinL2wordrecognitionandspellingskills,itisreasonabletoexpectthatitshouldplayamorepronouncedrolewhenwordandtextreadingefficiencyaretargeted.ThisissuehasreceivedverylittleattentionintheL2literatureandhasbeenlargelylimitedtothereadingper-formanceofadultL2readers(e.g.,Nassaji&Geva,1999;Segalowitz,1986;Shimron&Sivan,1994;Wade-Woolley&Geva,1999).Todate,onlyahandfulofL2studieshavesystematicallyexaminedthedevelopmentofwordandtexteffi-ciencyinL2children(e.g.,Geva&Clifton,1994;Geva,Wade-Woolley,&Shany,1997;Quiroga,Lemos-Britton,Mostafapour,Abbott,&Berninger,2002).ThestudiesbyGevaandhercolleaguesfocusedprimarilyonL1–L2transfer.Both
34
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
studieshaveshownthatprimary-levelpoordecoderswerelessfluentthangoodde-codersinreadingtextsinEnglish,intheirL1,andintheirL2(FrenchandHebrew,respectively).OfparticularrelevanceinthepresentcontextistheGevaetal.(1997)studyinwhichperformanceonbothaccuracyandspeedindexesofwordreadinginL1(English)andL2(Hebrew)wereverysimilar.Thisresultisconsis-tentwiththeobservationthatorallanguageproficiencydidnotmakeauniquecon-tributiontowordrecognitioninL2children.Inaddition,inaresultreplicatingJacksonandDonaldson(19),Gevaetal.reportedthatintheirL1(English),chil-dren’stextreadingtimewasfasterthanreadingthesamewordspresentedoutofcontext.However,contextdidnothaveafacilitatingeffectinHebrew,astheirHe-brew proficiency was minimal.
Questionsconcerningtheroleoflanguageproficiencyinenhancingreadingef-ficiencyareparticularlypertinentinL2contexts.CarlisleandBeeman(2000)re-portedthatprimary-levelSpanish–Englishbilingualchildren’svocabularyknowl-edgepredictedreadingcomprehensioninthesamelanguage.AnintuitivelysensiblehypothesisisthatwordrecognitionskillsofL2childrenshouldbeslowerthanthoseoftheirL1counterpartsbecausetheirvocabularyisnotasdevelopedandbecausetheymaybeslowerinaccessinglinguisticinformation(e.g.,phono-logical,semantic,morphological)thantheirL1counterparts.Thesamewouldbeexpected to be true for text reading fluency in young L2 learners.
ThequestionthenbecomestowhatextentareL1-basedtheoriesofreadingeffi-ciency,whichattributeslowerreadingtolessefficientwordrecognitionskillsandtoreducedaccesstolinguisticinformation,applicabletoL2learners?Areindivid-ualdifferencesinreadingefficiencyrelatedtoindividualdifferencesinunderlyingprocessessuchasphonologicalawarenessandrapidnamingjustastheyappeartobeinL1children?Alternatively,areoralproficiencyandtheabilitytobenefitfromlinguisticcontextthedrivingforcesinenhancingL2wordreadingefficiencyandtextefficiency?Accordingtothelatterexplanation,L2childrenwouldbeexpectedtohavelessefficientwordrecognitionskillsthantheirL1counterpartsandtobelessfluentreadersbecauseoflimitationsintheefficiencyofretrievingphonologi-cal, semantic, and grammatical information.
Insummary,althoughrecentresearchhasshedsomelightonthecognitive,lin-guistic,andorthographicunderpinningsofaccuratewordrecognitionskillsinL2children,adearthofknowledgeexistsaboutthedevelopmentofreadingefficiencyinL2children.Thisstudyfocusesontheemergenceofreadingefficiencyinyoung,primary-levelEnglish-as-a-second-language(ESL)childrenwhohavemasteredthebasicprinciplesofwordreading.Thestudy’sfirstobjectiveistocom-pareESLandEnglish-as-a-first-language(EL1)childrenonwordandtextreadingefficiencyandtoexaminetheeffectsofcontextonreadingefficiency.Acompari-sonofreadingefficiencyinEL1andESLchildrenwouldshedlightontheassump-tionthatEL1childrenshouldbemoreefficientreadersthanESLchildrenduetotheirbettercommandofthelanguage.Asecondobjectiveistoexaminetheextenttowhichasimilarsetofunderlyingprocessesdrivewordandtextreadingeffi-
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
35
ciencyinEL1andESLchildren.Toachievethisobjective,weexploretherelativecontributionofindividualdifferencesinorallanguageproficiency,underlyingcognitive-linguisticprocesses(e.g.,nonverbalability,rapidserialnaming,phono-logicalawareness),andwordrecognitiontotextandwordreadingefficiencyinEL1andESLchildren.Thethirdobjectiveistoexaminesimilaritiesanddiffer-ences in profiles of EL1 and ESL groups that differ in their reading efficiency.
METHOD
Participants
ESLandL1participantswererecruitedfromthreesubsequentcohortsofchildrenin12schoolsinfourschoolboardsinalarge,multiethnicmetropolisinCanada,where4of10residentswerebornoutsidethecountry.ItisnottheobjectiveofthisresearchtoexaminetheimpactofsocioeconomicstatusonreadingachievementinESLchil-dren.Moreover,wedidnothaveaccesstoindividualfamilylevelsocioeconomicsta-tusdata.The2001CanadianCensusdata1indicatethat,onaverage,anonofficiallan-guage(i.e.,neitherEnglishnorFrench)wasthelanguagespokenby58%ofthepeoplelivinginthecommunitiesfeedingintotheparticipatingschools(thepercent-agesrangedfrom43%to73%).Onaverage,68%ofthefamilieslivinginthesecom-munitiesimmigratedtoCanadawhentheywereatleast20yearsold(withlittlevari-abilitybetweenthecommunities);another23%indicatedthattheyimmigratedbetweentheagesof5and19.Inotherwords,themajorityoftheresidentswerefirst-generationimmigrants.Theincidenceofpoverty2variedsomewhatamongthecommunities.Whereasin2ofthecommunitiesnoincidenceofpovertywasre-ported,ineachoftheothercommunitiesacertainproportion(12%–50%)wereclas-sifiedaspoor;theaverageincidenceofpovertywas23%.Itisalsoimportanttonotethatthemedianfamilyincomeineachofthe12communitieswassubstantiallylowerthanthemedianreportedforthemetropolis.Anotherkeyindexofsocioeconomicstatusisparentaleducation.Quiteabitofvariabilityexistsintermsofthehighestlevelofeducationachieved,reflectingimmigrationtrendsandCanadianimmigra-tionpolicies.Onaverage,17%oftheindividualsinthecensusdistricthadlessthan9yearsofeducation,27%obtainedatleastsomesecondaryeducation,13%hadahighschoolcertificate,8%hadtraininginatrade,another19%hadcollegeeducation,and20%hadobtainedabachelor’sdegreeorahigheruniversitydegree.
ispossibletoaccessthecensusdemographicdatafordisseminationareasfromStatisticsCan-ada.Disseminationareasaresmall,relativelystablegeographicunits,composedofoneormoreblocks,whichrespecttheboundariesofcensussubdivisionsandcensustracts.Theyareuniformintermsofpopulationsize(400–700),andtheyarethesmalleststandardgeographicareaforwhichallcensusdataaredisseminated.Thedemographicinformationprovidedisbasedonthedatafromthedisseminationareas in which the participating schools are located.
2Poverty is defined as a family income of $30,000 (Canadian) or less for a family of four.
1It
36
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
Theinitialsampleconsistedof183ESLchildrenand70EL1children.Thedataof3(4.3%)EL1childrenand18(8.8%)ESLchildrenwhohadastandardscorebe-low80onameasureofnonverbalability(MatrixAnalogiesTest[MAT];Naglieri,19)werenotincludedintheanalyses,leavingfinalsamplesof183Grade2ESLchildren (46% girls) and 67 (62% girls) EL1 children.
ThenativelanguagegroupsrepresentedintheESLsampleareCantonese(19%),Punjabi(41%),Tamil(18%),andPortuguese(22%).TheESLchildrencomefrom12schools,andtheEL1childrenfrom7oftheseschools.Themeanagewas 88.43 months for the EL1 sample and 87.32 months for the ESL sample.ConsentformsinEnglishandchildren’shomelanguageweredistributedineachoftheparticipatingclassrooms.Onlychildrenwithparentalconsentpartici-patedinthestudy.Interviewsofparentsabouthomeliteracyandtheextenttowhichthenativelanguagewasusedathomewerenotconductedduetolanguagebarriers,budgetaryconstraints,andreluctancebytheschooldistrictstoallowac-cesstoparents.However,informationabouttheEL1orESLlanguagestatusofchildrenwasdeterminedthroughinformationabouteachchildrecordedinschoolfilesandparentresponsesincludedintheconsentforms.Thisinformationwassubsequentlyvalidatedduringinterviewswithclassroomteachers.OnlychildrenwhoseschoolrecordsandteacherinterviewsindicatedanESLstatuswereconsid-eredassuchinthestudy.Inaddition,childrenwhohadnotlivedinanEng-lish-speakingcountryforatleast4monthsattheonsetofGrade1werenotin-cluded.Thisprecautionwastakentoensurethatchildrenwhowereincludedhadsome systematic exposure to the rudiments of language and literacy instruction.InEnglish-speakingCanada,school-agechildrenwhoarerecentarrivalsfromanon-English-speakingcountrytypicallyattendschool-basedESLclassesforupto2years.Intheschooldistrictswherethisstudywasconducted,ESLinstruction,whichisprovidedonawithdrawalbasis,typicallyoccursindaily30-to40-minsessionswithgroupsofthreetofivechildren.InthesesmallgroupsallchildrenhavesimilarlevelsofEnglishlanguageproficiencybutnotnecessarilythesameL1.TeacherswithESLspecialisttrainingconducttheseclasses.ESLclassesfocusonthedevelopmentofspokenEnglishandonreadinessforliteracyskills.BesidestheESLtutoring,newimmigrantchildrenattendregularclassrooms,inwhichallinstruction takes place in English.3
Classroomteachersareexpectedtoprovideappropriateadaptationstothecurric-ulum.SomeoftheESLchildreninthisstudywereattendingESLclassesatthetimeoftestingorhadattendedsuchclassesintherecentpast.Exceptforpull-outsforsmall-groupESLtutoring,theESLchildrenwerecompletelyintegratedintothereg-ularclassroom.
inlargeurbancentersinCanadaareethnicallyandlinguisticallydiverse.Therefore,
providingsystematiclanguageandliteracyinstructioninthehomelanguageisnotfeasible.Insomecommunities,however,childrenmayattendheritagelanguageprograms.Typically,theseclassestakeplaces after school or on weekends.
3Classrooms
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
37
Measures
Cognitive and Linguistic MeasuresNonverbal intelligence.ChildrencompletedtheMAT(Naglieri,19),ameasureofnonverbalintelligence.Inthistest,childrenarepresentedwithanillus-trationofanincompletevisuo-spatialmatrixandaskedtocompleteitbylocatingthemissingpieceamongfiveorsixpatternedsegments.Thetesthasfoursubtests(patterncompletion,reasoningbyanalogy,serialreasoning,andspatialvisualiza-tion),eachofwhichconsistsof16matrixes.Testingwithineachsubtestisdiscon-tinued after four consecutive errors. Results are reported in standard scores.RAN.TheRANtask,developedbyDencklaandRudel(1976),wasusedtomeasurespeedofrapidserialnaming.Inthiscontinuousnamingtask,childrenareaskedtonamefivelettersasfastastheycan.Eachletterappears10timesinran-domorderwithin10setsoffiveitems.PriortoadministeringtheRANtask,thechildisaskedtonameeachofthefiveletterstoascertainfamiliaritywiththelet-ters.TheRANmeasureisnotadministeredtothosechildrenwhocannotnameallfiveletterswithoutassistance.Thechildren’stime(inseconds)tonameallthelet-tersontheboardisusedasthenamingspeedmeasure.Notethatthelowerthescore is the faster is the naming speed.
Phonological awareness (PA).PAwasmeasuredwithataskadaptedfromtheAuditoryAnalysisTaskdevelopedbyRosnerandSimon(1971).Inthisseg-mentation-deletiontask,childrenmustisolateanddeletesyllablesorphonemesandindicatetheresultingword(e.g.,“Saymeat.Nowsayitwithoutthe/m/.”).MethodologicalconsiderationsguidedusinadaptingthistasktotheESLpopula-tion.Inparticular,itwasnecessarytominimizethepossibleconfoundingoflan-guageproficiencywithperformanceonthisphonologicalawarenesstask.Ourpri-maryconcernwasthatmanyofthewordsortheresultingwordsontheoriginalAuditoryAnalysisTaskarenotlikelytobefamiliartoyoungESLchildren(e.g.,stale).Theitemsontheadaptedtaskareallhigh-frequencywords,andtheresult-ingwordsafterthechilddeletesthephonemeorsyllablearealsohigh-frequencywords(e.g.,“Sayleg.Nowsayitwithoutthe/l/.”).Ofthe25itemsonthetask,thefirst4involvesyllabledeletion,andtheremainderrequirephonemedeletioninwordinitial,wordfinal,orwordmedialposition.Someitemsrequirethedeletionofaphonemeinaconsonantcluster.Fourpracticeitemsprecedetheadministra-tionofthetestitems.Administrationisdiscontinuedafterfiveconsecutiveerrors.The total correct scores are reported. The Cronbachαis .92.
Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test–Revised.TheExpres-siveOne-WordPictureVocabularyTest–Revised(Gardner,1990)wasusedtoas-sesslanguageproficiency.Itisastandardizedmeasureofexpressivevocabularyinwhichchildrenareaskedtoprovideone-wordlabelstolinedrawingpicturespre-
38
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
sentedtothemoneatatime.Thetest,whichincludes100nounsandverbs,isdis-continuedonceacriterionofsixconsecutiveerrorshasbeenestablished.Thetotalnumber of correct responses was used in the analyses.
Grammatical judgment.Children’ssyntacticknowledgewasassessedus-ingitemsadaptedfromagrammaticaljudgmentmeasuredevelopedbyJohnsonandNewport(19).Inthis40-itemmeasureofreceptivesyntacticskills,thechildlistenstoprerecordedtapedsentencesthatareeithergrammaticallycorrect(e.g.,“Weatethewholepizzabyourselves.”)orincorrect(e.g.,“Januaryisthemostcoldmonthoftheyear.”).Halfofthesentencesaresyntacticallycorrect,andtheotherhalfaresyntacticallyincorrect.Eachsentenceisplayedtwiceonataperecorder,andthechildisaskedtoindicatewhetherthesentenceissaid“therightway”or“thewrongway.”Nodiscontinueruleisusedonthistest.Thetotalscorewasbasedon the number of correctly judged sentences. The Cronbachαvalue is .77.Basic Reading Skills MeasuresWord Attack.TheWordAttacksubtestoftheWoodcockReadingMasteryTest–Revised(Woodcock,1987)wasusedtoassesschildren’sabilitytoutilizetheirknowledgeofgrapheme–phonemecorrespondencerulesandorthographicrepresentationstodecodeor“attack”pseudowordsinEnglish.Thistestconsistsof50pronounceablepseudowordsthatcomplywithEnglishorthographicrules(e.g.,plip,cigbet).Childrenreadthepseudowordsoneatatime,andtestingisdiscontin-uedwhenthechildmakessixconsecutiveerrors.Resultsarereportedintermsoftotal correct scores.
Word Recognition.Toassesschildren’sEnglishwordrecognitionskillstheWordRecognitionsubtestoftheWideRangeAchievementTest–Revised(Jastak&Wilkinson,1984)wasused.Thistestconsistsof42unrelatedwords.Itbeginswithhighlyfamiliar,shortwords(e.g.,cat),andgraduallythewordsbecomelessfrequentandmorecomplexorthographically(e.g.,pseudonym).Testingisdiscon-tinuedwhenthechildmakes10consecutiveerrors.Resultsarereportedintermsoftotal correct scores.
Reading Efficiency MeasuresTheBiemillerTestofReadingProcesses(Biemiller,1981)wasusedtomeasurewordandtextreadingefficiency.Thetestyieldsmeasuresofaccuracyandspeedinreadingisolatedwordsandconnectedtext.Theisolatedwordlistscomefromthecorresponding texts.
Word efficiency.Thissubtest,whichrequireschildrentoreadisolatedwords,providesanindicationofchildren’sabilitytoidentifyunrelatedwordsac-curatelyandquickly.Childrenarepresentedwithtwolists,eachconsistingof50
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
39
randomlyordered,single-morphemewords(takenfromthetwocorrespondingtexts;seethefollowingdiscussion)andaskedtoreadthemasfastaspossible.Thefirst list has easier words than the second.
Text efficiency.TheTextReadingsubtestfocusesontextreadingefficiency.Participantsarepresentedwithtwonarrativetexts(containing100wordseach).Theyarerequiredtoreadeachtextaloudasquicklyastheycan.Thesubtestin-cludestwonarratives,an“easy”text,which,accordingtoBiemiller(1981)usesprimer-levelwords(e.g.,bear,thank,no,they,water,fish,tried,andfather),andarelativelymore“difficult”text,whichconsistsofwordsthataretypicalofmiddleelementary-levelreading(e.g.,register,asked,interested,saw,things,wood,tour-ists,andalso).Thereadingmaterialsareeasytominimizeconfoundingofspeedand accuracy (Jackson & Donaldson, 19).
Test administration.Thechildfirstreadstheeasytextandthenthecorre-spondingwordlist.Themoredifficulttextandthecorrespondingwordlistaread-ministerednext.Bothsetscontainwordsthatwerechosenbecausetheycanbede-codedwithminimaldifficulty.AsBiemiller(1981)pointedout,thegoalofthetestistoascertainnothowmanywordschildrencanreadbutratherthespeedatwhichtheyreadthesewords.Assessmentofreadingefficiency,however,canbecompro-misedifchildrenmaketoomanyerrors.Theassessoristhusinstructedtoaidthechildwhostumblesonawordbyprovidingwithoutdelaytheunknownwordaswellasthesubsequenttwowords.TheBiemillerteststipulatesthatifachildmakesmorethan25%errorsonthefirsttexttheyshouldnotbeadministeredthesecondtext.Accordingly,inthisstudy,whenchildrenfailedtoreadthefirstpas-sagewithanacceptabledegreeofaccuracy(75%accuracyregardlessofspeed),theywereconsideredtobepoordecodersandwerenotadministeredthesecondpassage and corresponding word list.4
Speed scoring.Toobtainaspeedscoreintheisolatedwordscondition,wedividedthetotalnumberofsecondsthataparticipanttooktoreadtheeasyandhardlistsbythetotalnumberofcorrectwordsreadacrossthetwolists.Thesameproce-durewasrepeatedtocalculatetextspeedscores.TheEL1andESLgroupsdidnotdifferonwordaccuracy(85%and88%,respectively)orontextaccuracy(%and90%, respectively).
comparisonofefficiencyscoresintheEL1andESLgroupswiththosepublishedbyBiemiller(1981)revealedthatthespeedscoresreportedbyBiemillerforthesameagegroupwerehigherthanthoseobtainedinthisstudy.However,Biemillerexcludeddataofchildrenwhomademorethanthreeerrorsonthetasksbecausehefocusedontherelationshipbetweenspeedandaccuracyinchildrenwhoreadwithaccuracy.TheobjectiveofthisstudyistocompareESLandEL1children,and,therefore,theexclusioncriterionwasdifferent—onlychildrenwhomademorethan25%errorswhentheyreadthefirst and easy text were excluded.
4A
40
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
Reading efficiency scores.Twodependentmeasureswerederived—awordefficiencyscoreandatextefficiencyscore.EfficiencyscoreswerecomputedbasedonaprocedureusedbyStanovichandWest(19).First,foreachpartici-pantthenumberoferrorsoneachtaskandthetimetoperformeachtaskwerecon-vertedintotheirrespectiveZscores.TheresultingaccuracyandspeedZscoresforeachtaskwerethencombinedandaveragedtoyieldasinglecompositeefficiencyscore.Thetwoword-basedcompositeefficiencyscores(basedontheeasyanddif-ficultlists)wereaveragedtoyieldanoverallscorethatrepresentsthechild’sabil-itytoreadwordsefficiently.Thesameprocedurewasappliedtodevelopanoveralltextefficiencyscorethatrepresentsthechild’sabilitytoreadwithaccuracyandspeedthetwonarratives.NotethatthelowertheZscore,themoreefficientthechild’s reading.
Procedures
Aspartofalargerproject(whichbeganwhenthechildrenwereinGrade1),chil-drenwereadministeredaseriesoftestsinthefirsthalfoftheir2ndyearinelemen-taryschool.AtthattimechildrenintheEL1andESLgroupshavespent1½to2½yearsinschoolsinwhichEnglishisthelanguageofinstruction(i.e.,seniorkinder-gartenandGrade1).Thesetaskswerepartofalargerbatteryoftests,whichwasadministeredacrossfourtestingsessions,eachlastingapproximately30min.Al-thoughthebatterieswereadministeredinarandomorder,testswithinthebatterieswereadministeredaccordingtoafixedrandomorder.Childrenweretestedonanindividualbasisbyoneofanumberofexperiencedgraduatestudents.FortheMAT, standardized scores were used. However, raw scores were used when otherstandardizedtestssuchastheExpressiveOne-WordPictureVocabularyTest(Gardner,1990)andtheWoodcockReadingMasteryTest–Revised(Woodcock,1987) were used, because the norms were not developed for ESL children.
RESULTS
Ofthe67EL1children,12(18%)weredeemedtobepoordecoders,astheymadetoomanydecodingerrorsontheeasystoryandwere,therefore,notabletoprog-resstothedifficultstory.Ofthe183ESLchildren,32(17%)werenotabletoprog-resstothedifficultstoryandwerelikewiseconsideredpoordecoders.Fifty-fourEL1and151ESLchildrenmettheminimumrequirementsforbecomingefficientreadersbecausetheywereabletocompleteboththeeasyanddifficultsubtasksoftheBiemiller(1981)test.Across-tabchi-squareanalysiswasdonetoexaminewhethertheproportionofpoordecoderswashigherintheESLgroupthanintheEL1group.ThisanalysisrevealsthattheproportionofpoordecodersintheEL1and ESL groups was similar,χ2(1,N= 250) = .07,p= .460.
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
41
ThefocusofthisarticleisonacomparisonofunderlyingprocessesassociatedwithreadingefficiencyinESLandEL1children.ThebulkoftheResultssectionfocusesonacomparisonofEL1andESLchildrenwhowereabletoreadbothsto-rieswithrelativeaccuracyandspeed.AttheendoftheResultssectionwerevisitthepoordecodersgroup,whenwecomparetheprofilesoftherelativelyefficientreaderswiththeprofilesofthepoordecoders.Bonferonicorrectionwasappliedtoadjustthealphalevelformultipleanalyses.Analphaof.01wasselectedastheminimum acceptable level.
Comparison of EL1 and ESL Groups on Cognitive,Language, and Reading Measures
Table1providessummarystatisticsbylanguagegroupassociatedwithallthecog-nitive,linguistic,reading,andoralproficiencymeasures.Amultivariateanalysis
TABLE 1
Differences Between EL1 and ESL Groups on Age, MAT, SyntacticKnowledge, Vocabulary, PA, and RAN:Descriptive Statistics and
Multivariate Analysis of Variance Summary Table
EL1a
MeasuresAge (months)MAT
GramJudgExpVocPARANWordRecWordAttWordAccTextAccWordSpTextSp
M88.43101.5029.6361.8112.0434.6611.0015.8384.7488.561.501.17
SD3.617.2.7611.534.747.714.508.6611.8215.45.71.55
M87.32101.0326.06.4213.6329.9812.0017.9587.67.661.13.99
ESLb
SD3.5.465.3414.806.156.444.2710.5911.9717.99.61.55
Language Group, Fc
3.790.1118.92***47.92***2.9918.85***2.111.732.400.14.02**4.83
Note.EL1=Englishasafirstlanguage;ESL=Englishasasecondlanguage;MAT=MatrixAnalogiesTest;PA=PhonologicalAwareness;RAN=RapidAutomatizedNaming(inseconds);GramJudg=GrammaticalJudgmentTest;ExpVoc=ExpressiveOne-WordPictureVocabularyTest;WordRec=TotalwordsreadcorrectlyonWideRangeAchievementTest–Revisedwordrecognition;WordAtt=TotalcorrectonPseudowordreading;WordAcc=%wordsreadcorrectlyon2Biemillerwordlists(outof100);TextAcc=%correctwordsover2Biemillertexts(outof200);WordSp=speedperword(inseconds)over2Biemillerwordlists(lowerisfaster);TextSp=speedperword(inseconds)to read 2 Biemiller texts (lower is faster).
an= .bn= 151.cAnalysis is based on low- and high-efficiency groups only.**p< .001. ***p< .0001.
42
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
ofvariance(MANOVA)revealedthatthelanguagegroupsdidnotdifferfromeachotherontheMAT,andtheMANOVAdidnotrevealsignificantdifferencesbe-tweenthetwolanguagegroupsonage.However,ascanbeseeninTable1,onthetwoorallanguageproficiencyindexes(ExpressiveOne-WordPictureVocabularyTestandtheGrammaticalJudgmenttask)themeanintheEL1groupwassignifi-cantlyhigherthanintheESLgroup.Inthisregard,itisusefultonoteaswellthatacomparisonofgroupmeanswithgradeequivalentnormsrevealedthattheExpres-siveOne-WordVocabularymeanintheEL1groupwaswithintheaveragerange(withameanageequivalentof7years10months),whereastheageequivalentscoreintheESLgroupwasabout2yearsbelowthatoftheEL1group(withameanage equivalent of 5 years 9 months).
Onthenamingspeedmeasure(i.e.,RAN),childrenintheESLgroupweresig-nificantlyfasterthantheirEL1counterparts.Onthephonologicalawareness(i.e.,PA)task,thewordrecognitiontaskandthewordattacktasknodifferencesexistedbetween the ESL and EL1 children.
AscanbeseeninTable1,despitethedifferencesinorallanguageproficiency,ESLandEL1groupsdidnotdifferonaccuracyindexesofbasicreadingskills(i.e.,wordrecognitionandwordattack).Basedonthetestnorms,thegroupmeansob-servedforboththeEL1andESLgroupswerewithinthenormalrangefortheirgradeinschool(gradeequivalent=2.1–2.2).Inthesamevein,acomparisonofthegroupmeansonthewordattacktaskrevealedthattheESLgroupwasslightlyhigher (grade equivalent = 2.2) than the EL1 group (grade equivalent = 1.9).ToexaminecontexteffectsonaccuracyandspeedintheEL1andESLgroups,two-wayanalysesofvariance(ANOVAs)wereconductedwithcontext(wordsvs.text)asarepeatedmeasureandgroup(EL1vs.ESL)asanonrepeated,independ-entvariable.Resultspertainingtoreadingaccuracyindicatedthatamaineffectex-istedforcontext,F(1,248)=.32,p<.0001,suggestingthatinbothlanguagegroupsreadingincontextwassomewhatmoreaccuratethanreadingisolatedwords.Thelanguagegroupeffectwasnotsignificant,indicatingthatthetwogroupsreadatthesamelevelofaccuracy.However,asignificantLanguageGroup×Contextinteractionexists,F(1,248)=6.59,p<.01,indicatingthatcontextwasmorefacilitatingintheEL1group.Resultsofthetwo-wayANOVApertainingtospeedshowthatasignificantmaineffectexistsforcontext,F(1,248)=162.26,p<.0001,indicatingthatinbothlanguagegroupstextswerereadfasterthanisolatedwords.Amaineffectwasalsofoundforlanguagegroup,F(1,248)=8.28,p<.005,indicatingthatESLchildrenwerefaster,andasignificantContext×Lan-guageGroupinteraction,F(1,248)=26.02,p<.0001,indicatingagainthatcon-text was more facilitating in the EL1 group than in the ESL group.
Tosummarize,theEL1groupoutperformedtheESLgrouponthelanguageproficiencymeasures,buttheESLgroupoutperformedtheEL1grouponlet-ter-namingspeed(i.e.,RAN)andword-namingspeed.ThegroupsdidnotdifferonPA,wordrecognition,andpseudowordreading.Finally,thecontexteffectwasmore pronounced in the EL1 group than in the ESL group.
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
43
Correlates of Reading Efficiency
ToexaminepatternsofindividualdifferencesinreadingefficiencyintheEL1andESLgroups,wefirstexaminedintercorrelationsamongthereadingefficiencyscoresandthepotentialpredictorvariables.Table2providesasummaryoftheseanalyseswithineachlanguagegroup.Ingeneral,itisnoteworthythatintheESLgroupmostvariablescorrelatedsignificantlywitheachother,whereasintheEL1groupthiscorrelationwasnotconsistent.Theresultsofthecorrelationalanalysescoupledwiththeoreticalconsiderationsguidedusintheselectionofpredictorvari-ablesinsubsequenthierarchicalregressionanalysesandindeterminingtheorderinwhichvariableswereenteredintotheregression.Inparticular,potentialpredic-torvariablesthatareofamoregeneralnature,suchasnonverbalabilityandlan-guageproficiencyindexes,wereenteredfirst.Thevariablesofinterest(i.e.,basicprocessingindicessuchasphonemicawarenessandnamingspeed)wereenteredinsubsequentsteps.Wordrecognitionwasenteredlasttoexaminetheextenttowhichaccuratewordrecognitionmakesauniquecontributiontoreadingeffi-ciencyoverandabovethecontributionofthegeneralandbasicprocessingvari-ableslistedabove.NotethepositiveandsignificantcorrelationbetweenExpres-sive One-Word Picture Vocabulary and Grammatical Judgment (r= .48).Predictors of Reading Efficiency
AscanbeseeninTable2,veryhighcorrelationsexistbetweenthetwo(untimed)basicwordreadingmeasures(wordattackandwordrecognition)andthewordandtextefficiencymeasures.Toavoidmulticolinearity,wordattackwasnotincludedintheregressionanalyses,butwordrecognitionwasenteredlastfortheoreticalreasons.Inparticular,thewordrecognitiontaskincludesalargearrayofwordsvaryinginfrequencyandregularity,anditcorrelateswithspellingandortho-graphicskills.Inthesehierarchicalregressionanalyses,conductedseparatelyfortheEL1andESLsamples,textefficiencyandwordefficiencywerethedependentmeasures.Inthefirstsetofregressionanalyses,allpotentialpredictorswerein-cluded.Insubsequentanalysesnonsignificantpredictorswereomittedifagivenvariable was not significant for both language groups.Predictors of Reading Efficiency in the ESL Group
ThefirstsetofhierarchicalregressionanalysesexaminedtheroleofMAT,oralprofi-ciency(asmeasuredbyvocabularyknowledgeandgrammaticaljudgment),phono-logical-processingskills(asmeasuredbyPAandRAN),andwordrecognitionskillsinexplainingindividualdifferencesinwordandtextreadingefficiencyintheESLgroup.Ingeneral,thepercentageofvarianceexplainedbyeachofthepredictorvari-ableswassimilarforboththewordandtextreadingefficiencymeasures.AscanbeseeninTable3,MATdidnotplayasignificantroleinexplainingvarianceinword
44
3–.121.368**1.000.367*.268–.216–.307–.250.346.381*.096.466***.479***1.000.248.108–.084–.014.2.127.001.356***.401***.293**1.000–.149–.327*–.279.562***.662***.033–.169–.147–.243*–.267**1.000.590***–.610***–.556***–.581***–.0–.193–.273**–.378***–.508***.604***1.000.966***–.798***–.680***–.090–.142–.239*–.337***–.493***.624***.967***1.000–.790***–.661***4567.029.339***.380***.416***.693***–.3***–.838***–.813***1.000.826***10.017.369***.345***.368***.731***–.447***–.739***.719***.850***1.000TABLE2IntercorrelationsAmongAge,MAT,LanguageMeasures,PA,RAN,WordEff,andTextEffintheEL1andESLGroupsESL12EL11.Age2.MAT13.GramJudg4.ExpVoc5.PA6.RAN7.TextEff8.WordEff9.WordRec10.WordAtt1.000.030–.112.065.199–.124–.073–.073.153.246–.225*1.000.280.298.184–.084–.238–.228.246.293Note.EL1n=,andESLn=151.EL1correlationsareabovethediagonalandESLarebelowthediagonal.MAT=MatrixAnalogiesTest;PA=phono-logicalawareness;RAN=RapidAutomatizedNaming;WordEff=wordefficiency;TextEff=textefficiency;EL1=Englishasafirstlanguage;ESL=Englishasasecondlanguage;GramJudg=GrammaticalJudgmentTest;ExpVoc=ExpressiveOne-WordPictureVocabularyTest;WordRec=totalwordsreadcorrectlyonWideRangeAchievementTest–Revised;WordAtt=totalcorrectonPseudowordReading.*p<.01.**p<.001.***p<.0001.READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
TABLE 3
Hierarchical Regression Analyses Predicting Reading Efficiency in ESL,
With MAT, Syntactic Knowledge, Vocabulary, PA, and RAN
as Independent Variables
Word Efficiency
VariableMAT
GramJudgExpVocPARANWordRec
R2.02.06.13.31..73
∆R2.02.04.07.18.23.19
∆F3.036.2110.7236.2169.0596.33
pns.014.001.0001.0001.0001
β.12.09–.08–.01.24–.72
R2.03.09.16.32.53.75
∆R2.03.05.07.16.20.22
Text Efficiency
∆F5.108.12.4333.9060.34120.76
pns.005.001.0001.0001.0001
β
45
.07.07–.09.03.19–.77
Note.ESL=Englishasasecondlanguage;MAT=MatrixAnalogiesTest;PA=phonologicalawareness;RAN=RapidAutomatizedNaming;GramJudg=GrammaticalJudgmentTest;ExpVoc=ExpressiveOne-WordPictureVocabularyTest;WordRec=totalwordsreadcorrectlyonWideRangeAchievement Test–Revised.
andtextreadingefficiencyintheESLgroup.Ofthetwoorallanguageproficiencymeasures,grammaticaljudgmentwasmarginallysignificant(p=.014),explaining4%ofthevarianceinwordreadingefficiency,anditwassignificant,explaining5%ofthevarianceinthecaseoftextreadingefficiency.Vocabulary,thesecondmeasureoforallanguageproficiency,wassignificant,explaininganadditional7%ofthevarianceinbothwordefficiencyandtextreadingefficiency.PAexplainedanaddi-tional18%ofthevarianceinwordreadingefficiencyand16%ofthevarianceintextreadingefficiency.RANaddedafurther23%totheexplainedvarianceinwordread-ingefficiencyand20%totextreadingefficiency.ItisnoteworthythatRANandPAexplainedtogether41%ofthevarianceinwordreadingefficiencyand37%ofthevarianceintextreadingefficiency,eventhoughtheywereenteredafterMATandtheoralproficiencyindexes.Finally,wordrecognition,enteredlast,explainedanaddi-tional19%ofthevarianceinwordreadingefficiencyand22%intextreadingeffi-ciency.Altogether,73%ofthevarianceinwordefficiency,and75%ofthevarianceintextefficiencywasexplainedbythepredictorvariables.Predictors of Reading Efficiency in the EL1 Group
AswasthecaseintheESLgroup,thehierarchicalregressionanalysesexaminetheroleofMAToralproficiency,PA,RAN,andwordrecognitioninexplainingwordandtextreadingefficiencyintheEL1group.DuetopowerconstraintsintheEL1sample,inthefirststepthepredictormeasureswereenteredinthreeblocks:MATinthefirstblock,thetwooralproficiencymeasuresinthesecondblock,andthetwophonological-processingmeasures(RANandPA)andwordrecognitioninthethird block.
46
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
TABLE 4
Hierarchical Regression Analyses Predicting Reading Efficiency in EL1,With MAT, Vocabulary, Syntactic Knowledge, Vocabulary, PA, and RAN
as Independent Variables
Word Efficiency
Text Efficiency
pns
R2.06
∆R2.06
∆F2.
pns
VariableBlock 1MATBlock 2
GramJudgExpVocBlock 3PARANWordRecPARANWordRec
R2.06
∆R2.06
∆F2.82
.13.081.99ns.14.092.27ns
.71.08.42.69.57.08.34.2827.374.0426.4440.08.0001ns.0001.0001.69.11.41.68..11.31.2724.125.6624.1037.08.0001ns.0001.0001
Note.EL1=Englishasafirstlanguage;MAT=MatrixAnalogiesTest;PA=phonologicalaware-ness;RAN=RapidAutomatizedNaming;GramJudg=GrammaticalJudgment;ExpVoc=ExpressiveOne-WordPictureVocabularyTest;WordRec=totalwordsreadcorrectlyonWideRangeAchieve-ment Test–Revised.
Resultsofthefirstsetofanalysespertainingtowordandtextreadingefficiency(seetoppanelofTable4)showthatonlythethirdblock—consistingofPA,RAN,andwordrecognition—wassignificant.Becausethefirsttwoblockswerenotsignif-icantforeitherwordortextefficiency,weeliminatedthesevariablesfromsubse-quentanalyses.ThelowerpanelofTable4providesasummaryoftheseresults.InthesecondsetofanalysesPA,RAN,andwordrecognitionwereenteredoneatatime.TheseanalysesrevealedthatPAwasnotsignificantforwordortextreadingeffi-ciency,andRANwashighlysignificant,explaininganadditional34%ofthevari-anceforwordefficiencyand31%fortextefficiency.Wordrecognition,enteredlast,explainedanadditional28%ofthevarianceinwordefficiencyand27%intexteffi-ciency.Altogether,62%ofthevarianceinwordefficiencyand58%ofthevarianceintextefficiencywereexplainedbyRANandaccuratewordrecognition.5
5Itshouldbenotedthatmultipleregressionanalyses,usingspeedofwordandtextreadingasade-
pendentvariables(ratherthanefficiencyscores)andtheidenticalsetofpredictorvariablesreportedinthetext,yieldedsimilarresultstothosereportedinthisarticle,withonlyslightfluctuationsintheper-centage of variance explained by each additional variable.
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
47
Bydefinition,itisnotpossibletomeasurereadingefficiencyinchildrenwhosereadingskillsarenotsufficientlydeveloped.Atthesametime,itisusefultoexam-ineprofilesofEL1andESLchildrentobetterunderstandtheunderlyingmecha-nismsthatdistinguishsubgroupsofchildrenthatdifferinreadingefficiency.Weturn to this topic in the next section.
Subgroup Reading Efficiency Profiles
TheefficientEL1andESLreaders(i.e.,thosewhocompletedbothtextsontheBiemiller,1981,task)weredividedfurtherintotwosubgroups,highefficiencyandlowefficiency,basedonthemedianofthereadingefficiencydistribution.Toex-cludeborderlinecasesaroundthecutoffpoint,dataofchildrenwhosetextreadingefficiencyZscoreswerebetween–.05and.05weredropped.Tothisend,thedataof6(9%)EL1participantswereexcluded,aswerethedataof5(3%)ESLpartici-pants.Thehigh-efficiencygroupconsistedofchildrenwhoseZscoreswereatleast.05abovethemedianonthetextreadingefficiencyindex.Thelow-efficiencygroupconsistedofchildrenwhoseZscoreswereatleast–.05belowthemedianonthetextreadingefficiencyindex.TheEL1low-efficiencygroupconsistedof20(30%)children,andtheEL1high-efficiencygroupconsistedof29(43%)children.TheESLlow-efficiencygroupconsistedof42(24%)children,andtheESLhigh-efficiencygroupconsistedof104(57%)children.Thethirdsubgroupinte-gratedintothisanalysisconsistedofthepoordecoderswhowerenotabletocom-
FIGURE1ESLandEL1profilesofpoordecoders,low-efficiency,andhigh-efficiencyread-ers (Zscores).
48
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
pletethetwostories.Thepoordecodersubgroupconsistedof12(18%)childreninthe EL1 group and 32 (17%) children in the ESL group.
Figure1providesavisualdepictionoftheEL1andESLhigh-efficiency,low-efficiency,andpoordecoderprofiles.Toexaminethereadingefficiencygroupandlanguagegroupeffectsonthecognitive,linguistic,andreadingmea-sures,aMANOVAwithSchefféposthocanalyseswascarriedout.TheresultsoftheseanalysesaresummarizedinTable5.Consistentwiththepreviousanaly-ses,asignificantlanguage–groupeffectexistsonthetwoorallanguageindexesaswellasonRANbutnotonMAT,PA,oranyofthereadingmeasures.Asig-nificantreadingefficiencysubgroupeffectoccursonallthemeasuresexceptfortheExpressiveOne-WordVocabularyTest,whichwasmarginallysignificant(p=.015).Posthocanalysesindicatedthatthepoordecodersubgroupandthelow-efficiencysubgroupweresignificantlylowerthanthehigh-efficiencygrouponMATandonthelanguageproficiencymeasures.TheefficiencygroupsalsodifferedfromeachotheronRAN,PA,wordrecognition,andwordattack.Theinteractionbetweenlanguagegroupandreadingefficiencygroupwasnotsignif-icantforanyofthevariables.
TABLE 5
The Effect of Language Group, Efficiency Group, and the Language Group× Efficiency Group Interaction on MAT, Syntactic Knowledge, Vocabulary,PA, RAN, Word Recognition, Word Attack, Word and Text Accuracy, andWord and Text Speed:Multivariate Analysis of Variance Summary Table
Language Group
Effect
F
MAT
GramJudgExpVocRANPA
WordRecWordAtt
1.0322.***84.17***26.41***0.050.260.76
Language ×EfficiencyEffect
F0.020.343.181.794.010.822.45
Efficiency Group Effect (a,b,c)aF8.03***8.28***5.67*68.88***31.80***165.07***83.24***
Post Hoca < c***; b < c**a < c**; b < c**a < c***; b < c**a < b < c***
a, b < c***; a < b**a < b < c***
a, b < c***; a < b**
Note.AllmeasuresconvertedtoZscores.MAT=MatrixAnalogiesTest;PA=phonologicalawareness;RAN=RapidAutomatizedNaming(inseconds);GramJudg=GrammaticalJudgment;ExpVoc=ExpressiveOne-WordPictureVocabularyTest;WordRec=totalcorrectonWideRangeAchievement Test–Revised; WordAtt = total correct on Pseudoword Reading.aa = poor decoders, b = low-efficiency readers, c = high-efficiency readers.*p< .01. **p< .001. ***p< .0001.
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
49
DISCUSSION
ESL and EL1 Differences on Cognitive-Linguistic andReading Measures
ResultsofthisstudyareinlinewithpreviousresearchthathasshownthateventhoughEL1andESLchildrendifferontheiroralproficiency,theycanperformatthesamelevelonaccuracyindexesofbasicreadingskillssuchaswordrecognitionandwordattack(e.g.,Gevaetal.,2000;Gottardoetal.,2001;Lesaux&Siegel,2003).Thisstudytakestheresearchinthisareaonestepfurtherbyexaminingreadingeffi-ciencyofESLchildrenattheprimarylevel.ResultsofthisstudysuggestthatESLchildrencanreadwordsandsimpletextswiththesameefficiencyasEL1children.ThesefindingschallengetheintuitivebeliefthatESLchildrenwouldreadisolatedwordsandtextlessefficientlybecauseoflowercommandoftheL2.
Surprisingly,theESLchildrenreadisolatedwordssignificantlyfasterthanthechildrenintheEL1group,andtheywerealsofasterthantheEL1grouponase-quentialletter-namingtask.ThisfindingisconsistentwiththefindingsofLesauxandSiegel(2003),whofoundanESLadvantageonnamingspeed.TheESLad-vantageonwordreadingspeedandletter-namingspeedcanbeapproachedfromthree different angles.
First,thesedifferencescouldbeattributedtosocioeconomicstatusfactors.However,childrenintheEL1andESLgroupsweresampledfromschoolslocatedinthesameworking-classneighborhoods,thusreducingthestrengthofthisargu-ment.Second,giventhatEL1andESLchildrenwereselectedfromthesameschools,theargumentthattheresultsmightreflectsystematicdifferencesinin-structionalapproachescanberuledout.Third,groupdifferencesinspeedcouldbeattributedtogroupdifferencesinabilityinoneoftwoways.Oneargumentisthatthegroupsmightdifferingeneralcognitiveability.However,resultsindicatethattheEL1andESLgroupsdidnotdifferonnonverbalaspectsofcognitiveability.Relatedly,somemightarguethatthedifferencesreflectthepositiveimpactofbi-lingualismonunderlyingbasiccognitiveprocessesthatareessentialforreadingdevelopment(Bialystok,2001;Bialystok&Herman,1999).Todate,thisargumenthasbeenexaminedwithregardtometalinguisticskills,andnosoundtheoreticalorempiricalevidenceexiststosuggestthatbilingualismmighthaveafacilitatingef-fect on basic naming speed processes.
InthiscontextitisimportanttobemindfulofCummins’s(2000)thresholdhy-pothesisthatattemptstoreconcileresearchevidenceonpositiveandnegativeef-fectsofbilingualism.Thishypothesissuggeststhatlanguageandcognitivedevel-opmentmightbeenhancedinbilinguals,providedthatrelativelyhighlevelsofproficiencyhavebeenattainedinbothlanguages.Clearly,thishypothesisisnotap-plicabletoyoungESLlearners.Noneoftheseexplanationsaresatisfactory,and
50
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
furtherresearchisnecessarytoexplorethefactorsthatmightcontributetotheESLadvantages, reported in this article and elsewhere (e.g., Lesaux & Siegel, 2003).ItisnoteworthythatESLchildrenaresimilartotheirEL1counterpartsintheirabilitytoreadsimplenarrativeswithaccuracyandspeed.Inotherwords,pri-mary-levelESLchildrencandevelopaccuracyandspeedinL2readingand,in-deed,achieveefficiencysimilartothatoftheirEL1counterparts,providedthattheyareexposedtosystematicinstructioninlanguageandliteracyinEnglish(Gersten&Baker,2003;Gersten&Geva,2003;Lesaux&Siegel,2003;Stuart,1999)andthattheyhavewell-developedwordrecognitionskills.AsChall(1996)remarked,inStage2mostchildrenlearntousetheirdecodingknowledgeanduti-lizethe“redundanciesofthelanguageandthestoriestheyread”(p.19),whichcanbethecaseforESLchildrenaswell,providedthatthelanguageofthenarrativesisnottoodemanding.Atthesametime,itisimportanttorememberthattheroutetra-versedbyEL1andESLchildrenisnotidentical:TheEL1childrencometoschoolalreadyequippedwiththelanguagetheyneedtoenablethemtoreadsimpletexts(Chall,1996),whereastheESLchildrendeveloptheirlanguageskillsinparalleltothedevelopmentoftheirreadingskills.Itisalsoimportanttobearinmindthat,notwithstandinghomelanguage,readingefficiencyisaconstructwhoseveryna-ture changes with development and that it is not an all-or-none phenomenon.Previousliteratureoncontexteffects(e.g.,Jackson&Donaldson,19)hasshownthatcontextenhanceswordreadingefficiency.Likewise,inthisresearchmonolingualandESLchildrenarefoundtoreadwordsfasterandmoreaccuratelywhenthewordsarepresentedincontextthanwhenthesamewordsarepresentedinisolation.However,thecontextfacilitationwasmorepronouncedintheEL1groupthanintheESLgroup,presumablyduetothefactthattheEL1childrenhavebettercommandoftheEnglishlanguagethantheESLchildren.Abettercommandofthelanguagehelpschildrentoanticipatewordspresentedincontextanden-hancesaccurateandfastrecognitionofprintedwords.Inastudyofprimary-levelchildren,Gevaetal.(1997)showedthatwhenchildrenreadintheirL1contextfa-cilitationisusedandwordsarereadmoreaccuratelyandfaster.However,thisfa-cilitationwasnotdetectedwhenthesamechildrenreadintheirL2(Hebrew),alan-guageinwhichtheyhadminimalproficiency.ThediscrepancybetweentheresultsoftheGevaetal.studyregardingthecontexteffectforL2readersmaybeduetodifferencesinmorphosyntacticcomplexity(Hebrewisdenser)andthefactthatthelevelofEnglishorallanguageproficiencyoftheESLparticipantswasmoread-vancedinthisstudythanwasthecaseintheGevaetal.study.Finally,futurere-searchneedstoexaminetheextenttowhichtheresultscanbereplicatedinade-signinwhichword-readinglatencyismeasuredforwordspresentedoneatatimeonacomputerscreen.JacksonandDonaldsonfoundthatprecociousyoungerread-ersweredisadvantagedontheBiemiller(1981)scrambledlistbutnotwhenword-readinglatencywasmeasuredwithwordspresentedindividuallyonacom-puterscreen.ItispossiblethatbecausetheEL1childrenmightbemoresensitiveto
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
51
grammaticalpatternsandgrammaticalexpectanciesthantheESLchildren,thereadingofascrambled,seriallypresentedlistismoredebilitatingtothemthantoESL children.
Factors Contributing to Reading Efficiency in EL1and ESL Children
Thesecondobjectiveofthisstudyistoexaminetherelativecontributionoforallanguageproficiency,underlyingcognitive-linguisticprocesses,andwordrecog-nitiontoreadingefficiency.Thisobjectiveembracestworelatedquestions:Whataretheprocessingfactorscontributingtowordandtextreadingefficiency,andtowhat extent does the same pattern exist in the ESL and EL1 groups?
Anintuitivelyappealingargumentmightbethattoreadwordsefficientlychil-drenneedtohavewell-developedvocabularyandwell-developeddecodingskillsbutthatsyntacticanddiscourselevelknowledgeisnecessarytoreadtextswithef-ficiency.However,theresultsdidnotrevealadifferentpathforwordandtextread-ingefficiencyintheEL1orESLgroups.IntheESLgroupvocabularyandsyntac-ticknowledgeexplainedjointly11%ofthevarianceinthecaseofwordefficiencyand12%inthecaseoftextefficiency.TheseresultssuggestthattheESLpartici-pantsreachedsomekindofathresholdintheirEnglishlanguageproficiencythatenabledthemtoreadwitheasetextsthatdonotchallengetheircurrentlinguisticknowledge.Thatis,itappearsthatwhenthelanguageusedinthereadingmaterialsisbeloworperhapsjustattheleveloforalproficiency(Chall,1996),L2oralprofi-ciencycontributesonlymarginallytowordortextreadingefficiencyofESLchil-dren,anditplaysnoroleinthecaseofEL1children.Thescenariomightbediffer-entwhenthereadingmaterialsaremoredemandingintermsoftherangeofvocabularyandsyntacticstructures.Thisissueneedstobeexploredinfuturere-search.Moreresearchisneededtoexaminethecontributionoforallanguageprofi-ciencywhenlinguisticallymoredemandingtextsarepresented.Furthermore,inthisresearchaccuracyaspectsoforallanguageproficiencyweretapped.Theex-tenttowhichorallanguagefluencymightcontributemoresubstantiallytoreadingefficiency is another question that should be targeted.
Resultsofthisstudydemonstratethatoverandabovethecontributionoforallanguageproficiency,RAN,PA,andwell-developedwordrecognitionskillscon-tributesubstantiallytowordandtextreadingefficiencyofESLchildren.RANandPAarethesameprocessingconstructsthathavebeenshownelsewhere(e.g.,Gevaetal.,2000;Gholamain&Geva,1999;Lesaux&Siegel,2003)toaccountforindi-vidualdifferencesinaccuratewordrecognitionskillsofEL1andESLchildren.Inthisresearchweexaminetheroleoftheseprocessesaswellaswordrecognitioninreadingefficiency.TheresultsshowthattherolesplayedbyPA,rapidnaming,andaccuratewordrecognitionskillsinaccountingforindividualdifferencesinwordandtextefficiencyarenotidenticalforEL1andESLchildren.Eachofthesevari-
52
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
ablescontributessubstantiallytoreadingefficiencyinESLchildren.InthecaseofEL1children,rapidnamingandwordrecognitionplayasubstantialroleinwordand text efficiency, whereas PA does not.
Onewayofthinkingabouttheseresultsisthat,infact,ESLchildrenaremorechallengedbythereadingtaskspreciselybecausetheyreadinalanguageinwhichtheyarenotfluent.Therefore,theyneedtodrawonallthecognitiveandlinguisticresourcesthattheyhave.ThisexplanationissupportedbytheobservationthatintheESLgroup(butnotintheEL1group)mostofthecognitive,linguistic,andreadingmeasurescorrelatedsignificantlywitheachother.Thatis,thoseESLchil-drenwhoaremoreefficientreadersaremorelikelyalsotohavebetterlanguageskills,fasternamingspeed,betterdevelopedPAskills,andmoreadvancedwordrecognitionskills.ItisinterestingtoconsiderpossibletheoreticalunderpinningsofthedifferencesintheamountofvarianceexplainedbyrapidnamingandPAineachlanguagegroup.WeproposethatperhapstheseEL1–ESLdifferencesreflectthatinESLlearnerstheeffectofunderlyingcognitive-linguisticprocessesismoregenericormoreextensive.Itislikelythatwithincreasedlanguageproficiencyandadditionaldevelopmentofmoreadvancedreadingskillstheseunderlyingpro-cessesmaygraduallybecomemorespecializedanddomainspecific,thewaytheyare in the EL1 group. This hypothesis is worth exploring in future research.
Ingeneral,youngESLschoolchildrenwhohaveacquiredacertainleveloforallanguageproficiencyinEnglishandwhodevelopedtheiroralandliteracyskillsinanimmersioncontextcanberathersimilartotheirEL1counterpartsintermsoftheeasewithwhichtheycanexecutebasicwordrecognitionprocessesandreadeffi-cientlysimplenarratives.Clearly,aspectsoflanguageproficiencysuchaslexicalandgrammaticalskillsareessentialforreadingefficiency;theESLchildrenwhowereefficientreadersalsohadhigheroralproficiencyinEnglishthanthoseESLchildrenwhowerelessefficient.Atthesametime,simplisticnotionsofL2readingperformancethatemphasizeprimarilyorallanguageproficiencyneedtobere-fined.Thisrefinementcanbeachievedbyconsideringthecognitive-linguisticpro-cessesthatunderliereadingprocessesandthatappeartobesourcesofindividualdifferencesinefficientreadingofEL1andESLchildren.LessefficientreadinginESLandEL1childrenalikecanbeattributedtolessefficientwordrecognitionskills and to reduced or less effective access to linguistic information.Comparing the Profiles of ESL and EL1Efficiency Subgroups
L1-basedresearchsuggeststhatnotallchildrenwhoreachGrade2arereadytode-veloptheirreadingefficiencyandthatsomecontinuetobe“dysfluent”(Wolf&Katzir-Cohen,2001).ResultsofthisstudyextendthisstatementtoESLchildrenintheprimarylevel.TheexaminationofprofilesofEL1andESLsubgroupsthatdif-ferintheirreadingefficiencyunderscorestherangeoflanguageandreadingskills
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
53
thatexistamongESL(andEL1)learnersandprovidesusefulinsightsintothetypeofreadingskillsthatEL1andESLchildrendevelopandthecognitiveandlinguis-tic resources they draw on to read with efficiency.
Severalobservationscanbemadeonthebasisofanexaminationoftheeffi-ciencygroupprofilesdepictedinFigure1.Mostimportant,thesubgroupsdefinedonthebasisofreadingefficiencyareconsistentlydifferentfromeachotheroncog-nitive,orallanguage,andreadingmeasures.Next,clearly,regardlessofefficiencygroup,EL1childrenhavebetterdevelopedorallanguageskillsinEnglishthanESLchildren.However,thelanguagegroupeffectconcerningrapidnamingisinthereversedirection,withESLchildrenwithineachefficiencygroupoutperform-ingtheirEL1counterparts.(Possibleexplanationsforthisfindinghavebeenprevi-ouslydiscussedwithregardtoESL–EL1differencesonrapidautomatizedletternaming.)Third,itisofinterestthatESLchildrenwhowereclassifiedashigh-effi-cientreadershadsignificantlyhigherscoresonPAthanthehigh-efficientEL1children,whereasamonglow-efficientandpoordecoders,EL1childrenoutper-formedtheirESLcounterparts.Becausethehigh-efficiencyEL1andESLgroupsdidnotdifferonnonverbalability,anexplanationforthisunexpectedresultbasedongeneral,nonverbalcognitiveabilitycanberuledout.However,possiblythisESLadvantageinthehigh-efficientgroupmayberelatedtotheenhancingeffectsofbilingualism.Itispossiblethatthehigh-efficientESLchildrenbenefitfromtheirbilingualstatusinawaythatdistinguishesthemfromESLchildrenwhoarelessefficientreaders.Theymaybebetterabletoextractsubtlephonologicaldis-tinctionsandhavemoreprecisephonologicalrepresentationsthatmayberelatedalsototheirrelativelybetter-developedvocabulary(Metsala&Walley,1998).Finally,itisinformativetonotethatthehigh-efficiencyESLchildrenhadbetterdevelopedorallanguageproficiencyinEnglishthantheESLchildrenwhowerepoor decoders or low-efficiency readers.
Thereareclinicalandresearchimplicationstotheseobservations.WhenyoungESLchildrenhaveproblemsindevelopingreadingefficiencyevenwhentheyreadsimplematerials,attributiontolackofadequateorallanguageproficiencyisnotwarrantedautomatically—especiallywhenESLchildrendeveloptheiroralandlit-eracyskillsinasystematicreadinginstructionprogram.Theculpritinthiscasemaybeinaccurateanddysfluentwordrecognitionskillsanddeficitsintheunder-lyingcognitive-linguisticprocessesnecessaryforthedevelopmentofaccurateandfluentwordrecognitionskills.Inadditiontoorallanguagedevelopment,suchchil-drenmaybenefitfrominterventionapproachesthatfocusonthedevelopmentofefficientwordrecognitionskills(Gersten&Baker,2003;Gersten&Geva,2003;Quiroga et al., 2002).
FutureresearchshouldcontinuetoexplorethedevelopmentofreadingefficiencywithL2childrenofdifferentages,withdifferentlevelsoforallanguageproficiency,andwithacademicreadingmaterialsthatvaryinlinguisticandorthographicde-mands.Specialattentionisalsoneededtoexaminetherelevanceofthesefindingsto
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
childrenwho,inadditiontobeingL2learners,mayhavealearningdisabilityoralan-guageimpairment.Thisresearchwasconductedinurbanandsuburbanareasthatareethnicallyandlinguisticallymixed,butsomeimmigrantcommunitiesaremoreho-mogeneousinsocioeconomicstatusandhomelanguage.Toexaminethegeneralizabilityoftheseresults,large-scale,multilevelstudiesneedtobeconductedthatconsidertheeffectsofwithin-childfactorsandcontextualfactors,includinglevelofethnicdiversityatthecommunityandschoollevelforlanguageandliteracyoutcomes.Finally,giventhelargenumberofchildrenwholearntoreadinanL2withouttheoptionoffirstmasteringtheirL1readingskills,questionspertainingtoL2readingefficiencyandtotheimpactofdifferenteducationalpracticesonreadingefficiencyandonreadingcomprehensionofacademicandnarrativetextsdeservesustainedandsystematicattentioninthenearfuture.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Thisresearchwassupportedbygrant410–96–0851fromtheSocialSciencesRe-searchCouncilofCanadaandagrantfromtheOntarioMinistryofEducationtoEstherGeva.ZohrehYaghoubZadehisananalystattheCanadianCouncilonLearning.
WethankthestaffandchildrenatthePeelBoardofEducation,theTorontoDis-trictSchoolBoard,theTorontoSeparateSchoolBoard,andtheYorkRegionSepa-rate School Board of Education for their patience and cooperation.
REFERENCES
Arab-Moghaddam,N.,&Sénéchal,M.(2001).OrthographicandphonologicalprocessingskillsinreadingandspellinginPersian/Englishbilinguals.InternationalJournalofBehavioralDevelop-ment, 25,140–147.
Berninger,V.W.,Abbott,R.D.,Billingsley,F.,&Nagy,W.(2001).Processesunderlyingtimingandfluencyofreading:Efficiency,automaticity,coordination,andmorphologicalawareness.InM.Wolf(Ed.),Time, fluency, and dyslexia(pp. 383–414). Timonium, MD: York.
Bialystok,E.(2001).Bilingualismindevelopment:Language,literacy,andcognition.NewYork:Cambridge University Press.
Bialystok,E.,&Herman,J.(1999).Doesbilingualismmatterforearlyliteracy?Bilingualism:Lan-guage and Cognition, 2,35–44.
Biemiller,A.J.(1977/78).Relationsbetweenoralreadingratesforletters,words,andsimpletextsinthe development of reading achievement.Reading Research Quarterly, 13,223–253.
Biemiller,A.J.(1981).BiemillerTestofReadingProcesses.Toronto,Ontario,Canada:UniversityofToronto Press.
Bowers,P.,Golden,J.,Kennedy,A.,&Young,A.,(1994).Limitsuponorthographicknowledgeduetoprocessesindexedbynamingspeed.InG.Berninger(Ed.),Thevarietiesoforthographicknowledge:I.Theoreticalanddevelopmentalissues(pp.173–218).Dordrecht,TheNetherlands:KluwerAcademic.
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
55
Bowers,P.G.,&Wolf,M.(1993).Theoreticallinksamongnamingspeed,precisetimingmechanismsand orthographic skill in dyslexia.Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5,69–85.Breznitz,Z.(2001).Theroleofinter-modalitytemporalfeaturesofspeedofinformationprocessinginasynchronybetweenvisual-orthographicandauditory-phonologicalprocessing.InM.Wolf(Ed.),Dyslexia, fluency and the brain(pp. 245–276). Timonium, MD: York.
Breznitz,Z.(2002,June).BrainactivityofdyslexicandnormalreadersduringL1andL2taskperfor-mance.PaperpresentedattheinternationalConferenceonMultilingualandCross-CulturalPerspec-tives on Dyslexia, Washington, DC.
Carlisle,J.F.,&Beeman,M.M.(2000).Theeffectsoflanguageofinstructiononthereadingandwrit-ingachievementoffirst-gradeHispanicchildren.JournalofScientificStudiesofReading,4,331–353.
Carver,R.P.(1997).Readingforonesecond,oneminute,oroneyearfromtheperspectiveofraudingtheory. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1,3–43.
Chall, J. S. (1996).Stages of reading development(2nd ed.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace.
Chiappe,P.,&Siegel,L.S.(1999).PhonologicalawarenessandreadingacquisitioninEnglish-andPunjabi-speaking Canadian children.Journal of Educational Psychology, 9,20–28.
Comeau,L.,Cormier,P.,Grandmaison,E.,&Lacroix,D.(1999).Alongitudinalstudyofphonologicalprocessingskillsinchildrenlearningtoreadinasecondlanguage.JournalofEducationalPsychol-ogy, 91,29–43.
Denkla,M.B.,&Rudel,R.G.(1976).Rapid‘automatized’naming(R.A.N.):Dyslexiadifferentiatedfrom other learning disabilities.Neuropychologia, 14,471–479.
Durgunoglu,A.Y.,Nagy,W.E.,&Hancin-Bhatt,B.J.,(1993).Cross-languagetransferofphonologicalawareness.Journal of Educational Psychology, 85,453–465.
Ehri,L.(1998).Grapheme-phonemeknowledgeisessentialforlearningtoreadwordsinEnglish.InJ.Metsala&L.Ehri(Eds.),Wordrecognitioninbeginningreading(pp.3–40).Mahwah,NJ:LawrenceErlbaum Associates, Inc.
Gardner,M.(1990).ExpressiveOne-WordPictureVocabularyTest–Revised.Novato,CA:AcademicTherapy Publications.
Gersten,R.,&Baker,S.K.(2003).English-languagelearnerswithlearningdisabilities.InH.L.Swanson,K.R.Harris,&S.Graham(Eds.),Handbookoflearningdisabilities(pp.94–109).NewYork: Guilford.
Gersten,R.,&Geva,E.(2003).TeachingreadingtoEnglishlearnersintheprimarygrades:InsightsintothenewresearchbaseonteachingreadingtoEnglishlearners.EducationalLeadership,60,44–49.Geva,E.,&Clifton,S.(1994).ThedevelopmentoffirstandsecondlanguagereadingskillsinearlyFrench immersion.Canadian Modern Language Review, 50,6–667.
Geva,E.,&Siegel,L.S.(2000).Orthographicandcognitivefactorsintheconcurrentdevelopmentofbasicreadingskillsintwolanguages.ReadingandWriting:AnInterdisciplinaryJournal,12,1–31.Geva,E.,Wade-Woolley,L.,&Shany,M.(1997).Developmentofreadingefficiencyinfirstandsecondlanguage.Scientific Studies of Reading, 1,119–144.
Geva,E.,YaghoubZadeh,Z.,&Schuster,B.(2000).Understandingindividualdifferencesinwordrec-ognition skills of ESL children.Annals of Dyslexia, 50,123–1.
Gholamain,M.,&Geva,E.(1999).Orthographicandcognitivefactorsintheconcurrentdevelopmentof basic reading skills in English and Persian. Language Learning, 49,183–217.
Gottardo,A.,Yan,B.,Siegel,L.S.,&Wade-Woolley,L.(2001).FactorsrelatedtoEnglishreadingper-formanceinchildrenwithChineseasafirstlanguage:Moreevidenceofcross-languagetransferofphonological processing.Journal of Educational Psychology, 93,530–2.
Jackson,N.E.,&Donaldson,G.(19).Precociousandsecond-gradereaders’useofcontextinwordidentification.Learning and Individual Differences, 1,255–281.
Jastek,S.,&Wilkinson,G.S.(1984).WideRangeAchievementTest–Revised.Wilmington,DE:JastakAssociates, Inc.
56
GEVA AND YAGHOUB ZADEH
Johnson,J.S.,&Newport,E.L.(19).Criticalperiodeffectsinsecondlanguagelearning:Theinflu-enceofmaturationalstateontheacquisitionofEnglishasasecondlanguage.CognitivePsychology,21,60–69.
Kame’enui,E.J.,Simmons,D.C.,Good,R.H.,&Harn,B.A.(2001).Theuseoffluency-basedmea-suresinearlyidentificationandevaluationofinterventionefficacyinschools.InM.Wolf(Ed.),Time, fluency, and dyslexia(pp. 307–331). New York: York.
Kirby,J.,Parrila,R.K.,&PfeifferS.L.(2003).Namingspeedandphonologicalawarenessaspredic-tors of reading development.Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,453–4.
Kuhn,M.R.,&Stahl,S.A.(2000).Fluency:Areviewofdevelopmentalandremedialpractices.AnnArbor: University of Michigan, Center for the Improvement of Early Reading Achievement.
LaBerge,D.,&Samuels,S.J.(1974).Towardatheoryofautomaticinformationprocessinginreading.Cognitive Psychology, 6,293–323.
Lesaux,N.,&Siegel,L.S.(2003).ThedevelopmentofreadinginchildrenwhospeakEnglishasasec-ond language (ESL).Developmental Psychology, 39,1005–1019.
Lesaux,N.,&Siegel,L.S.(2004,March).PredictorsofreadingfailureinchildrenfrombothEnglishandnon-Englishspeaking(ESL)backgrounds.SixthBDAInternationalConference,UniversityofWarwick, United Kingdom.
Levy,B.A.(2000).Wholewords,segments,andmeaning:Approachestoreadingeducation.InR.Klein&P.McMullen(Eds.).Convergingmethodsforunderstandingreadinganddyslexia(pp.77–110). Cambridge. MA: MIT Press.
Lindsey,K.A.,Manis,F.R.,&Bailey,C.E.(2003).Predictionoffirst-gradereadinginSpanish-speak-ing English-language learners.Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,482–494.
Lipka,O.(2003).ThedevelopmentofreadingskillsofchildrenwithEnglishasasecondlanguage.Un-published doctoral dissertation. University of British Columbia, Vancouver.
Manis,F.R.,Seidenberg,M.S.,Doi,M.L.,Mcbride-Chang,C.,&Patterson,A.(1996).Onthebasisoftwo subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58,157–195.
Metsala,J.L.,&Walley,A.C.(1998).Spokenvocabularygrowthandthesegmentalrestructuringoflexicalrepresentations:precursorstophonemicawarenessandearlyreadingability.InJ.L.Metsala&L.C.Ehri(Eds.),Wordrecognitioninbeginningliteracy(pp.–120).Mahwah,NJ:LawrenceErlbaum Associates, Inc.
Meyer,M.S.,&Felton,R.H.(1999).Repeatedreadingtoenhancefluency:Oldapproachesandnewdirections.Annals of Dyslexia, 49,283–306
Naglieri, J. (19).Matrix Analogies Test.New York: Psychological Corporation.
Nassaji,H.,&Geva,E.(1999).CognitiveandlinguisticprocessesinadultL2readers.AppliedPsycholinguistics,20,241–267.
NationalReadingPanel.(2000).Teachingchildrentoread:Anevidence-basedassessmentofthescien-tificresearchliteratureonreadinganditsimplicationsforreadinginstruction(NIHPub.No.00–4769).Bethesda,MD:NationalInstitutesofHealth,NationalInstituteofChildHealthandHu-man Development.
Perfetti, C. A. (1985).Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.
Quiroga,T.,Lemos-Britton,Z.,Mostafapour,E.,Abbott,R.D.,&Berninger,V.W.(2002).Phonologi-calawarenessandbeginningreadinginSpanish-speakingESLfirstgraders:Researchintopractice.Journal of School Psychology, 40,85–111.
Rosner,J.,&Simon,D.(1971).Theauditoryanalysistest:Aninitialreport.JournalofLearningDis-abilities, 4,40–48.
Savage,R.,(2004).Motorskills,automaticityanddevelopmentaldyslexia:Areviewoftheresearchlit-erature.Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17,301–324.
Shany,T.M.,&Biemiller,A.(1995).Assistedreadingpractice:Effectsonperformanceforpoorread-ers in grades 3 and 4.Reading Research Quarterly, 30,382–395.
READING EFFICIENCY IN ESL CHILDREN
57
Shimron,J.,&Sivan,T.(1994).Readingproficiencyandorthography:EvidencefromHebrewandEnglish.Language Learning, 44,5–27.
Stage,S.A.,Sheppard,J.,Davidson,M.M.,&Browning,M.M.(2001).Predictionoffirst-graders’growthinoralreadingfluencyusingkindergartenletterfluency.JournalofSchoolPsychology,39,225–237.
Stanovich,K.E.(1992).Speculationsonthecausesandconsequencesofindividualdifferencesinearlyreadingacquisition.InP.B.Gough,L.C.Ehri,&R.Treiman(Eds.),Readingacquisition(pp.307–342). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Stanovich,K.,&West,R.(19).Exposuretoprintandorthographicprocessing.ReadingResearchQuarterly, 24,402–433.
Stuart,M.(1999).Gettingreadyforreading:Earlyphonemeawarenessandphonicsteachingimprovesreadingandspellingininner-citysecondlanguagelearners.BritishJournalofEducationalPsychol-ogy, 69,587–605.
Torgesen,J.K.(2001).Thetheoryandpracticeofintervention:Comparingoutcomesfrompreventionandremediationstudies.InA.Fawcett(Ed.),Dyslexia:Theoryandgoodpractice(pp.185–202).London: Whurr.
Wade-Woolley,L.,&Geva,E.(1999).Processinginflectedmorphologyinsecondlanguagewordrec-ognition:Russian-speakersandEnglish-speakersreadHebrew.ReadingandWriting:AnInterdisci-plinary Perspective, 11,321–343.
Wade-Woolley,L.,&Geva,E.(2000).ProcessingnovelphonemiccontrastsintheacquisitionofL2word reading.Scientific Studies of Reading, 4,267–293.
Wade-Woolley,L.,&Siegel,L.S.(1997).ThespellingperformanceofESLandnativespeakersofEng-lishasafunctionofreadingskills.ReadingandWriting:AnInterdisciplinaryJournal,9,387–406.Wagner,R.K.,Torgesen,J.K.,Rashotte,C.A.,Hecht,S.A.,Barker,T.A.,Burgess,S.R.,etal.(1997).Changingrelationsbetweenphonologicalprocessingabilitiesandword-levelreadingaschildrende-velopfrombeginningtoskilledreaders:A5-yearlongitudinalstudy.DevelopmentalPsychology,33,468–479.
Wang,M.,&Geva,E.(2003).SpellingacquisitionofnovelEnglishphonemesinChinesechildren.Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16,325–348.
Wimmer,H.,Mayringer,H.,&Landerl,K.(2000).Thedoubledeficithypothesisanddifficultiesinlearning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92,668– 680.
Wolf,M.,&Bowers,P.G.(1999).Thedouble-deficithypothesisforthedevelopmentaldyslexias.Jour-nal of Educational Psychology,91,415–438.
Wolf,M.,Bowers,P.,&Biddle,K.(2000).Naming-speedprocesses,timing,andreading:Aconceptualreview.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33,387–407.
Wolf,M.,&Katzir-Cohen,T.(2001).Readingfluencyanditsintervention.ScientificStudiesofRead-ing, 5,211–238.
Woodcock,R.W.(1987).WoodcockReadingMasteryTests–Revised.CirclePines,MN:AmericanGuidance Services.
Young,A.,&Bowers,T.G.(1995).Individualdifferenceandtextdifficultydeterminantsofreadingfluency and expressiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20,428–4.
Manuscript received April 23, 2004Accepted April 19, 2005
因篇幅问题不能全部显示,请点此查看更多更全内容
Copyright © 2019- igat.cn 版权所有 赣ICP备2024042791号-1
违法及侵权请联系:TEL:199 1889 7713 E-MAIL:2724546146@qq.com
本站由北京市万商天勤律师事务所王兴未律师提供法律服务